Toxicological evaluation of some food additives including anticaking agents, antimicrobials, antioxidants, emulsifiers and thickening agents WHO FOOD ADDITIVES SERIES NO. 5 The evaluations contained in this publication were prepared by the Joint FAO/WHO Expert Committee on Food Additives which met in Geneva, 25 June - 4 July 19731 World Health Organization Geneva 1974 1 Seventeenth Report of the Joint FAO/WHO Expert Committee on Food Additives, Wld Hlth Org. techn. Rep. Ser., 1974, No. 539; FAO Nutrition Meetings Report Series, 1974, No. 53. L-GLUTAMIC ACID AND ITS AMMONIUM, CALCIUM, MONOSODIUM AND POTASSIUM SALTS Explanation These substances have been evaluated for acceptable daily intake by the Joint FAO/WHO Expert Committee on Food Additives (see Annex 1, Ref. No. 23) in 1970. Since the previous evaluation, additional data have become available and are summarized and discussed in the following monograph. The previously published monograph has been expanded and is reproduced in its entirety below. BIOLOGICAL DATA BIOCHEMICAL ASPECTS There is evidence of rapid absorption of dietary glutamate since in rats the glutamic acid level in portal blood rose within 1/2 to 3/4 hour to 250% in adults and 150% in young animals over the testing level (Wheeler & Morgan, 1958). L-glutamic acid absorption by the dog failed to increase noticeably the amino-N2 of the peripheral blood but increased that of portal blood, possibly because of increased uptake by tissue (Christensen et al., 1948). Groups of eight rats were given by gavage 200 mg/kg bw of MSG alone or with 2000 mg/kg raw veal. Blood samples were taken at 10 minute intervals and after 30 minutes the animals were killed and free glutamic acid determined in blood and brain. Plasma glutamic acid rose rapidly to a peak in 20 minutes if monosodium glutamate was given alone and more slowly to a peak in 30 minutes if given with veal. Using 100, 500 and 2500 mg/kg orally produced a dose-related increase in plasma level only at the two higher test levels and more if monosodium glutamate was given alone. There was no effect on the brain glutamate acid levels. Using s.c. 500 mg/kg bw produced the same plasma levels as oral feeding. There was no adaptation. Brain levels were not affected. 100 mg/kg monosodium glutamate was the threshold dose before plasma levels rose. There was great variability in the response (McLaughlan et al., 1970). Continuous infusion of dogs with glutamic acid (0.5-4 mg/kg/hr) did not result in entry of glutamic acid into liver and muscle cells, cerebrospinal fluid or brain. Kidney cells appeared to be freely permeable. Metabolism of the infused glutamic acid was limited (Kamin & Handler, 1950). The intact rat as well as rat liver and rat tissues metabolize glutamate by oxidative deamination (von Euler et al., 1938) or transamination to oxaloacetic or pyruvic acid (Cohen, 1949) via alphaketoglutarate to succinate (Meister, 1965). This was shown by the use of 2-C14-labelled DL-glutamic acid given i.p. and resulting in the production of aspartic acid labelled in the -COOH radicals and glutamic acid labelled in position 1-C and 2-C. Intracaecally administered 2-C14-labelled DL-glutamic acid is rapidly converted to acetate, labelled in the methyl group, by the mesaconate and citramalate cycle. After gastric intubation of 2-C14-labelled DL-glutamic acid part is absorbed and metabolized to succinate, the rest to methyl-labelled acetate (Wilson & Koeppe, 1959). Rat tissue has only a poor ability to oxidize D-glutamate. After i.p. or s.c. administration conversion to D-pyrrolidone carboxylic acid occurs. Rat liver and rat kidney also convert enzymatically D-glutamic acid to D-pyrrolidone carboxylic acid (Wilson & Koeppe, 1961). The specific enzyme was isolated from the liver and kidney of mice, rats and man (Meister et al., 1963). Oral administration of monosodium L-glutamate (2 g/kg) to weanling rats caused a marked increase in the specific activity of liver carbamyl phosphate synthesase. Prolonged administration resulted in a return to control values, indicating an adaptation to the administered substrate (Hutchinson & Labby, 1965). Biochemical aspects are summarized in recent reports (Ajinomoto Co., 1970). I.v. injection of C14-labelled glutamic acid into intact rats and mice showed it to enter rapidly the brain, liver, kidney and muscle as such (Lajtha et al., 1959). Glutamic acid was shown to be distributed among more than one metabolic pool as animals mature (Berl, 1965). Compartmentation of glutamate metabolism in the mouse brain has been demonstrated by examining the time course of C14 incorporation into glutamine and glutamate (Van den Berg et al., 1969). Fifty patients with circulatory hypoxia received orally 1 g three times a day of glutamic acid for one week. All patients showed less blood lactic acid, a better alkali reserve and clinical improvement (Gorbunova et al., 1960). 15 g equivalents of un-neutralized L-glutamic acid, L-glutamic acid-HCl and monosodium glutamate were given orally to man. There was little absorption of the poorly soluble L-glutamic acid with very slight elevation of blood levels within one hour from 20 to 80 mg/l. Little absorption occurred with L-glutamic and acid-HCl, but monosodium glutamate was well absorbed, the blood level rising in one hour from 20 to 350 mg/l (Himwich, 1954). Six to 10 male healthy children were given 1, 2 and 4 g sodium glutamate. Total creatinine excretion was not affected but the amino acid/creatinine ratio increased much more than the glucuronic acid/creatinine ratio. Values returned to normal within 36 hours after 1-2 g and within 69 hours after 4 g (Inoue, 1960). Sodium glutamate has been used therapeutically in uraemia to reduce blood levels of ammonia. 8 g of i.v. glutamic acid caused nausea and vomiting in 11 from 17 individuals (Smyth et al., 1947). Introduction of 0.15% glutamate solution into the small intestine of the dog did not cause a rise in glutamate concentration in the blood draining the intestinal loop. Only at 0.5% did the venous blood contain extra glutamate. However, alanine appears in high concentration in the portal blood. When this mechanism is overwhelmed then glutamate appears also over and above the arterial blood level (Neame & Wiseman, 1957). In the cat and rabbit in vivo the same phenomenon occurs (Neame & Wiseman, 1958) and in the rat in vitro (Matthews & Wiseman, 1953) and in vivo (Peraino & Harper, 1962). Further removal of excess portal glutamate and alanine occurs in the liver. In man only two out of four subjects given 0.1 g/kg glutamic acid as a 7% solution orally showed an appreciable rise of free glutamic acid in plasma. Hence a similar mechanism may operate. Additional glutamic acid, e.g. 10-20 g if given to man, may raise the amount of glutamic acid absorbed from ingested protein (Bessman et al., 1948). Bound glutamate from proteins and polypeptides is released gradually during digestion and would be absorbed as alanine into the portal blood (Wiseman, 1970). L-glutamic acid and DL-glutamic acid are absorbed orally by the rat to nearly the same extent, L- being a little better absorbed (Aroskar & Berg, 1962). The fetal circulation has a higher amino acid concentration than the maternal in the rhesus monkey (Kerr & Waisman, 1967). Monosodium glutamate (8 g/kg bw) was administered orally to pregnant Wistar-Imamichi rats on day 19 of gestation. Plasma glutamic acid was determined in mothers and fetuses, at 30, 60 and 120 minutes after dosing. In the mothers' plasma, glutamic acid increased from approximately 100 µg/ml to 1650 µg/ml in the first 30 minutes. At the end of the test period the level was 1000 µg/ml. No significant changes occurred in the plasma glutamic acid of fetuses during this period (approximately 50 µg/ml) (Ohara et al., 1970a). Other effects observed were a decrease in tryptophan and tyrosine metabolism of the liver following daily injection of 1-4 g/kg glutamic acid into rats (Fumiwake, 1957). A reduction in the activity of liver catalase in mice after single injections of D-glutamic acid at 1.5 mg/g reverting to normal after four days and not observed with L-glutamic acid (Ando, 1959) enhanced oxygen consumption by rats after injection of 1 mg/kg sodium glutamate at low pO2, not observed at normal pO2 (Genkin & Udintsev, 1957) and hyperglycaemia after i.p. glutamate in rats due to conversion to glucose and additional stimulation of gluconeogenesis (Marcus & Reaven, 1967). Rats (Wistar-Imamichi), male adults (11-14 weeks of age) and male neonates (two to three days of age), were dosed orally with monosodium glutamate (0.5-8 g/kg bw for adults, and 0.5-4 g/kg bw for neonates). Plasma glutamic acid was measured over a four-hour period. For adult rats, the highest level tested showed maximum plasma glutamic acid, 1650 ug/ml, after 30 minutes. At the other dose levels there were no appreciable changes in plasma glutamic acid. In the case of neonates, levels rose to a maximum of 350 µg/ml after 90 minutes at the 2 g/kg dose level, and 1850 µg/ml after 90 minutes at the 4.0 g/kg dose level. In a similar study with mice (4CS strain) plasma glutamic acid of adults rose to a maximum of 530 µg/ml after 30 minutes, at the 2 g/kg dose level, and 1050 µg/ml at the 4 g/kg dose level. Neonate mice showed maximum level of plasma glutamic, 700 µg/ml 30 minutes after treatment at the 2 g/kg dose level, and 2300 µg/ml, two hours after treatment at the 4 g/kg dose level (Ichimura et al., 1970c). Another study showed that there was a marked correlation between liver SOT, SPT and plasma glutamic acid of rats and mice dosed orally with 1 g/kg bw monosodium glutamate. Measurements were made during the period 1-100 days of age (Hashimoto et al., 1970). When monosodium glutamate (1 g/kg bw) or monosodium glutamate (1 g/kg bw) plus powdered milk (1.5 g/kg bw) or powdered milk (1.5 g/kg bw) was administered orally to 10-day-old rats, the maximum levels of plasma glutamic acid were 425 µg/ml, 160 µg/ml and 105 µg/ml respectively. These levels occurred 30 minutes after dosing (Ohara et al., 1970b). L-monosodium glutamate-3-4-14C was administered to pregnant rhesus monkeys and serial maternal and fetal plasma samples analysed. In maternal plasma, 68% of the radioactivity remained in association with the glutamate, 22% was converted to glucose and smaller amounts into lactate, aspartate, glutamine and ornithine. In fetal blood, glucose and lactate accounted for more than 80% of the radioactivity with less than 2% of the label found in glutamate. Although maternal glutamate levels increased 25-fold, fetal levels were unchanged. Labelled glutamate administered to the fetus was not transferred to maternal circulation until fetal plasma glutamate levels reached 1000 µmoles/100 ml (normal 5 µmoles/100 ml) (Stegink et al., 1973). Levels of glutamic acid averaged 1.3-1.6 mg/g in the brain and 0.02-0.03 mg/ml in plasma of adult rat, mouse, guinea-pig and rabbit. In suckling rats glutamic acid in the brain increased from 0.5 mg/g at one day of age to 0.8 mg/g at seven days, 1.1 mg/g at 15 days and 1.3 mg/g at 21 days, whereas the levels in plasma decreased slightly from 0.04 mg/ml at one day, 0.03 mg/ml at 7-15 days and 0.02 mg/ml at 21 days. In suckling guinea-pigs there was only a slight rise in brain levels, from 1.3 mg/g at one day to 1.5 mg/g at 21 days, and no change in plasma levels. In adult rats glutamic acid levels averaged 1.53 mg/g brain and 0.02 mg/ml in plasma after three days' starvation, 1.61 mg/g and 0.02 mg/ml respectively after three days' starvation and one day normal feeding, 1.66 mg/g in brain and 0.54 mg/ml in plasma after three days' starvation plus a glutamate load of 10 mg/kg bw orally. Adult mice were given an oral load of MSG (10 g/kg bw). The glutamate levels in the brain averaged 1.28-1.43 mg/g during the subsequent 14 hours. The levels in plasma averaged 0.02 mg/ml at 0 hour, 1.45 mg/ml at one hour, 0.32 mg/ml at two hours, 0.05 mg/ml at four hours, and 0.03 mg/ml at 14 hours after treatment. Sprague-Dawley rats, body weight 175 g, were given an oral load of MSG, 10 g/kg bw. Glutamic acid levels in brain averaged 1.37-1.50 mg/g during the subsequent 14 hours, in plasma 0.02 mg/ml at 0 hour, 0.36 mg/ml at one hour, 0.30 mg/ml at two hours, 0.26 mg/ml at four hours, 0.12 mg/ml at six hours and 0.03 mg/ml at 14 hours after treatment. Rabbits, body weight 2.5 kg, were given an oral load of MSG, 10 g/kg bw. Glutamic acid levels in brain averaged 1.49-1.57 mg/kg during the subsequent six hours, in plasma they averaged 0.03 mg/ml at 0 hours, 0.37 mg/ml at one hour, 0.28 mg/ml at four hours and 0.16 mg/ml at six hours. Rats were given a subcutaneous injection of MSG at five days of age, 5 g/kg bw. Levels of glutamic acid in the brain averaged 0.66 mg/g at 0 hour, 0.76 mg/g at one hour, 0.73 mg/g at three hours, 0.90 mg/g at five hours and 0.96 mg/g at 14 hours, in plasma 0.03 mg/ml at 0 hours, 1.64 mg/ml at 1 hour, 0.41 mg/ml at three hours, 0.10 mg/ml at five hours and 0.03 mg/ml at 14 hours (Garattini, 1971). NUTRITIONAL ASPECTS L-glutamic acid occurs as a common constituent of proteins and protein hydrolysates and can be synthesized by the rat and rabbit from acetate fragments. Human plasma contains 4.4-4.5 mg/l of free glutamic acid and 0.9 mg/100 ml of bound glutamic acid. Human urine contains 2.1-3.9 µg/mg creatinine of free glutamic acid and 200 µg/mg creatinine of bound glutamic acid (Peters et al., 1969). Human spinal fluid contains 0.34-1.64 (mean 1.03 mg/l) free glutamic acid (Dickinson & Hamilton, 1966). Human milk contains 1.2% protein of which 20% is bound glutamic acid which is equivalent to 3 g/l calculated as sodium glutamate. The free glutamic acid concentration is 300 mg/l. In contrast cows milk contains 3.5% protein equivalent to 8.8 g/l calculated as MSG, but only 30 mg/l free glutamic acid (Maeda et al., 1958; 1961). Strained infant foods provide 80 Cal/100 g with wide variations depending on the recipe, while human milk provides 70 Cal/100 g (United Kingdom Dept. Health & Soc. Sec., 1970). Free amino acids were analysed by GLC in fruits and fruit juices. High levels of aspartic acid were found in figs (2.6 g/kg), nectarine (2.0 g/kg), peaches (1.1 g/kg), yellow plums (1.8 g/kg), dry prunes (1.9-5.2 g/kg). High levels of glutamic acid were found in cantaloupe (0.5 g/kg), grapes (0.4 g/kg) (Fernandez-Flores et al., 1970). Free glutamic acid was analysed in various foodstuffs by paper electrophoresis. Fish and meat had less than 0.1 g/kg, sausage 0.1-1.5 g/kg, cheese 0.2-22 g/kg, "tomatenflocken" 15 g/kg and dried mushrooms 17 g/kg (Müller, 1970). Infant food may contain up to 0.4% added MSG, the natural content depending on the basic constituents. Carrots contribute 0.32% free glutamic acid calculated as MSG, tomatoes, 0.45% and cheese 0.7%. Substitution of some unprepared foods in equal weights for prepared baby foods containing 0.3% added monosodium glutamate would also result in an ingestion of greater amounts of glutamate than is provided by mothers' milk on a calorie for calorie basis. The level of 0.3-0.5% in prepared foods appears to be the current level of use, since higher concentrations impart an unpleasant flavour. Infants aged three days and weighing 3 kg consume 480 g mothers' milk per day. This is equivalent to a daily intake of 1.104 g bound glutamic acid, and 0.115 g free glutamic acid corresponding to 0.408 g/kg bw of glutamic acid per day. One month old infants, weighing 3.8 kg consume 600 g mothers' milk per day. This is equivalent to a daily intake of 1.37 g of bound glutamic acid and 0.144 g free glutamic acid corresponding to 0.405 g/kg bw of glutamic acid per day. Infants aged five to six months, weighing 7.5 kg, consume 500 g cows' milk and two jars of baby food per day. The respective daily intake of bound glutamic acid amounts to 3.5 g and 0.5 g. The corresponding free glutamic acid intake is 0.015 g and 0.060 g per day, which is equivalent to 0.62 g/kg bw of glutamic acid per day. If the two jars (200 g/jar) contain 0.3% MSG, this increases the total intake of free glutamic acid from 0.06 to 0.60 g. In a seven day survey of children aged 9-12 months the intake of baby foods has been observed to range from zero (in 20% of the surveyed cases) to a maximum of 250 g daily in which up to 12 different preparations may be represented and not all of which have monosodium glutamate added (Berry 1970). Nutritional studies in the rat have shown glutamic acid to be a non-essential amino acid replaceable by others and to be required in substantial amounts to ensure high growth rates in rats (Hepburn et al., 1960). Some interconversion between glutamic acid and arginine can occur to cover minor dietary deficiencies (Hepburn & Bradley, 1964). Gouty patients have raised levels of plasma glutamate compared with normals and following a protein meal glutamate reaches excessive levels (Pagliari & Goodman, 1969). Premature and full-term infants hydrolyse any given protein in the stomach to very similar extents (Berfenstam et al., 1955). Hepatic glutamate dehydrogenase appears at 12 weeks of human fetal life, is present in rat fetal liver on day 17 and reaches its maximum within two weeks after birth (Francesconi & Villee, 1968). Male weanling rats were fed casein or purified amino acid diets for 14 or 21 days. The addition of 0.33% glycine or 1.14% glutamic acid to a diet with a protein equivalent (No. 6.25) of 10% essential amino acids increased food efficiency (g weight gain/g food) from 0.38 ± 0.01 to 0.41 ± 0.01 (Adkins et al., 1967). The nutritional value of non-essential amino acids as the nitrogen source in a crystalline amino acid diet for the chick growth was examined. The basal diet contained 26.8% of an essential amino acid mixture and an additional 24% of non-essential amino acids. In the test diets all non-essential amino acids were removed, and single amino acids were added as follows: glutamic acid 11.3%, aspartic acid 10.2%, alanine 6.8%, glycine 5.8%, proline 8.7% (additional to the 1% in basal diet), or serine 8.1%. Of these, glutamic acid and aspartic acid were found to be very useful nitrogen source, alanine was useful, glycine and proline were insufficient, and serine was harmful for chick growth. The chicks fed on the L-glutamic acid diet showed less growth than those fed on the basal diet, though there was no statistical significance between the groups (Sugahara & Ariyoski, 1967). The glutamate ion has a specific flavour effect that is more readily detected than NaCl when sodium chloride and MSG are added at approximately equal sodium concentrations (3-6 ml) to chicken broth containing normal amounts of NaCl (0.5-0.75%). MSG could readily be detected in the chicken broth at 0.04%. The ease of detecting glutamate is not greatly influenced by variation in levels of thickening agent, fat content or seasonings that would normally be encountered in foods. Added glutamate was much more difficult to detect in foods normally containing very high levels of glutamate (> 0.2%) than in those with very low levels of glutamate (> 0.02%). The glutamate content of the foods must be considered in developing recommendations for levels of added glutamate (Hanson et al., 1960). TOXICOLOGICAL STUDIES Special studies Special studies on multigeneration, reproduction and teratology Mouse The 4CS strain and Swiss white strains were studied. Groups of six mice (three male, three female), were maintained on diets containing 0%, 2% (= 4 g/kg/day) or 4% (= 8 g/kg/day) monosodium glutamate. Mice were mated after two to four weeks on the test diet. Offspring (F1) were weaned at age 25 days, and fed the same diet as parents. At age 90 days, selected (F1) male and female mice from each group were allowed to produce a single litter (F2). Parent mice were maintained on test diets, for 100 days after delivery and F1 mice for 130 days of age. F2 mice were reared until 20 days of age. No effects were observed on growth, feed intake, estrous cycle, date of sexual maturation (F1 generation), organ weight, litter size and body weight of offspring, and histopathology of major organs (including brain and eyes) of parent and F1 generation. Day of eye opening, general appearance and roentgenographic skeletal examination of F2 generation showed no abnormalities (Yonetani et al., 1970). Rat Six groups of 5-6 male and 5-10 female rats received by oral intubation daily 25 mg/kg or 125 mg/kg bw of glutamic acid monohydrochloride. Males and females received the compound during days 5-19 of one month, days 20-31 of the following month and days 1-10 during the third month. No adverse effects were noted on weight gain, feed intake or sexual cycles of females. No organ weight changes were seen in females but males on the higher dose level had enlarged spleens. Animals were mated at the end of the experiment and pups were normal (Furuya, 1967). Rats were given thalidomide combined with 2% L-glutamic acid and showed essentially the same defects in the pups as groups treated with thalidomide alone. A group receiving L-glutamic acid alone was no different from controls (McColl et al., 1965). Four females and one male fed for seven months on either 0%, 0.1%, 0.4% of monosodium L-glutamate, monosodium DL-glutamate or L-glutamic acid were mated and number of pups per litter was similar in all groups. Only 15-20% survived because of cannibalism. No abnormalities regarding fertility were seen on mating other groups of four females and one male at nine and 11 months. The F1 generation was mated at 10 months and an F2 generation produced in most groups but only the groups at 0.1 and the 0.4% L-glutamic acid produced an F3 and F4 generation. No impairment of fertility was noted (Little, 1953a). Monosodium glutamate was administered orally in doses up to 7 g/kg/day to pregnant rats on 6-15 or 15-17 days following conception. It produced no adverse effect in the progeny up to the period of weaning. Further physical development to maturity was also normal except that the progeny obtained from gravida treated on the 15-17 days during gestation showed impaired ability to reproduce (Khera et al., 1970). Two female rats received 4 g/kg bw of monosodium glutamate commencing at day 1 of pregnancy. There was no effect on pregnancy or lactation. Pups were divided into three groups. Two groups were nursed by parents receiving monosodium glutamate, and one group by untreated parents. At weaning (day 20), one group of pups that had been nursed by a parent receiving monosodium glutamate received approximately 5 g/kg monosodium glutamate daily for 220 days. Parents received 4 g/kg monosodium glutamate for 336 days. No effects were observed on growth or oestrus cycle. All pups developed normally, and no abnormalities were noted in growth rate, time of sexual maturity, oestrus cycle and fertility (Suzuki & Tagahashi, 1970). For histological studies, brain, hypophysis and eye were fixed in 10% neutral buffered formalin. Sections were stained with Hematoxylin- Eosin and Luxol fast blue-cresyl echt violet. No differences were observed between arcuate nuclei, medium eminence of hypothalmus and retina of control and monosodium glutamate treated groups (Shimizu & Aibara, 1970). Groups of female rats were maintained on diets containing 0.5%, 1% or 2% vitamin level. At each vitamin level diets also contained monosodium glutamate at 0, 1% or 2%. Reproductive performance of the rats (P) as well as the F1 offspring maintained on similar diets was studied. The addition of monosodium glutamate to the diet resulted in an increased fertility rate as well as increased survival at weaning of the offspring of the F1 generation. Addition of monosodium glutamate to the diet had no effect on growth rate in the neonatal period. Analysis of the brain tissue of first and second generation offspring at birth for RNA, DNA, protein, nucleus number and cellular size, showed that the brains of rats born of mothers (P) on monosodium glutamate diets contained a smaller number of nuclei and larger cells than controls. In contrast offspring of the F1 generation showed increased RNA, DNA and nucleus numbers when compared with the offspring of the P generation. The differences present at birth disappear at weaning (Semprini et al., 1971). Rabbit In one group of 10 female and four male rabbits only the females received orally 25 mg/kg bw of glutamic acid for 27 days. Two of the females were pregnant and the others were not pregnant. A second group of four female and two males received orally 25 mg/kg glutamic acid with 25 mg/kg vitamin B6. A third group of six females and two males received orally 25 mg/kg glutamic acid alone. A fourth group of 20 females and eight males served as controls. The test substance was given by gavage. The first group showed two animals with delayed pregnancy, the uterus containing degenerate fetuses. Two others had abortions of malformed fetuses. Two animals delivered at the normal time but the pups had various limb malformations. Four animals did not conceive. The pups did not become pregnant before seven months and showed limb deformities, decreased growth and development compared with controls. The histopathology showed scattered atrophy or hypertrophy of different organs. The second group produced two pregnant females which delivered malformed pups. These died soon after birth and showed bony deformities as well as atrophic changes in various organs. The third group produced three pregnant females which delivered pups with limb deformities. All three groups showed testicular atrophy in parents and multiple changes in the pups (Tugrul, 1965). Four groups of rabbits (24 females and 16 males) received either 0, 0.1%, 0.825% or 8.25% of monosodium glutamate in their diet for two to three weeks before mating. A positive control group of 22 pregnant females received 100 mg/kg thalidomide from day 8 to 16 of pregnancy. All does were sacrificed on day 29 or 30 of gestation and the uteri and uterine contents were examined. All males were sacrificed and the gonads and any abnormal organs examined. No significant effect on body weight gain or food consumption was seen, nor on general appearance and behaviour. Gross and histopathology revealed no toxic effects on embryos, resorption and pups and all litter data were comparable among test animals and negative controls (Hazelton Laboratories, 1966). The brains of five female and five male pups at the 8.25% level were subsequently checked for neuronal necrosis compared with controls, but none was found (Hazelton, 1969a). Similar investigations on five male and five female pups at the 0.1 and 0.825% levels were also negative (Hazelton, 1969b; Ebert, 1970). In another experiment on rabbits, these animals received 2.5 mg/kg bw, 25 mg/kg and 250 mg/kg of L-glutamic acid hydrochloride at 70 hours post coition and 192 hours post coition. Operative removal of fetuses was performed on the eleventh, seventeenth and thirtieth day post coition in three different series. The corporal lutea, the resorbed, implanted, normal and deformed fetuses were examined. No significant effects due to L-glutamic acid were noted with respect to teratogenesis (Gottschewski, 1967). Glutamic acid hydrochloride in a dose of 25 mg/kg bw was given orally to 15 pregnant rabbits once a day for a period of 15 days after copulation, monosodium glutamate in the same dose and for the same period of time to nine pregnant rabbits and saline solution to 11 pregnant rabbits which served as control group. No differences were noted between the treated groups and the controls as to rate of conception, mean litter size, and nursing rate. The average body weight of the young in the treated groups was slightly lower as compared with the control group, but the weights of testes, ovaries and adrenal glands in the young and ovaries, adrenal glands, liver, kidneys and spleen in the mothers were not different from those in the controls. In the young, no external and skeletal malformations were observed. There were some abnormal changes in gestation such as abortion or resorption of fetuses, but these observations were made in all groups, with an incidence of 21% in the glutamic acid hydrochloride group, of 25% after administration of monosodium glutamate, and of 27% in the controls. There were no external and skeletal malformations in the aborted fetuses (Yonetani, 1967). Chick embryo Fertilized hen eggs were incubated after a single injection of 0.01-0.1 mg glutamic acid into the yolk sac. The mortality of embryos was raised compared with controls (53% against 24% and there was a higher incidence of developmental defects (24% against 3%) especially depression of development of the spine, pelvis and lower limbs (Aleksandrov et al., 1965). In another study many variables were studied such as route of injection, dose and time of injection. No obvious toxicity or teratogenicity was observed (United States Food & Drug Administration, 1969). Special studies on mutagenicity Cells (kangaroo-rats cell line) were exposed continuously for 72 hours at 0.1% monosodium glutamate without showing any toxic effect (United States Food & Drug Administration, 1969). Groups of 12 male albino mice (Charles River strain), received a single oral dose, by gavage, of monosodium glutamate at levels of 0, 2.7 and 5.4 g/kg bw. The treated animals were mated with groups of three untreated females for each of six consecutive weeks. Females were sacrificed at mid-term of pregnancy, and the uterus examined for signs of early embryonic death. Females that had mated with treated males had numbers of implantations, resorptions, and embryos similar to those mated with controls (Industrial Bio-Test, 1973). Special studies on neurotoxicity L-glutamate and gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) supposedly act respectively as excitatory and inhibitory transmitters in the central nervous system. The glutamate-GABA metabolic pathway can use up a substantial fraction of the total oxygen consumed by the brain, and this may reflect the large amount of energy needed to maintain an adequate supply of these essential transmitters. Thus although cerebral glutamate first became well known because of its effects on the oxygen consumption and the sodium and potassium contents of the brain, these are probably secondary manifestations of transmitter functions. Glutamate is also involved in the synthesis and breakdown of cerebral proteins (Krnjevic, 1970) The level of 30 ninhydrin reactive compounds was studied in the brain of chick embryos (8, 14, 19, 20 and 21 days old) and chicks (1, 4, 8, 15, 30 days old and adult). Only glycerophosphoethanolamine, glutathione (GSH), aspartate, glutamine, glutamate and gamma- aminobutyric acid (GABA) increase during development. In later stages only the levels of GSH, aspartate, glutamate, glutamine and GABA remain as high as on the first day after hatching (Levi & Morisi, 1971). Of the free amino acids found in extracts of cat spinal roots, dorsal root ganglia and peripheral nerves, glutamate was present in disproportionally high concentrations in the parts of the dorsal roots between ganglia and spinal cord (Duggan & Johnston, 1970; Johnson & Aprison, 1970). The distribution of glutamate and total free amino acids in 13 specific regions of the cat was investigated. The highest levels of free glutamate were found in different cortical areas and in the candate nucleus. No such gradient was noted for total free amino acids in the same grey areas. The glutamate and total free amino acid content of the central grey tissues was always higher than that of the central white areas (Johnson & Aprison, 1971). The metabolism of glutamic acid in the brain is dependent on glucose and oxygen. By formation of glutamine it may influence ammonia levels in the brain. Most important is the formation of gamma- aminobutyric acid (GABA) by decarboxylation, a reaction which is dependent on pyridoxal phosphate, the coenzyme of glutamic acid decarboxylase (Perrault & Dry, 1961). The effect on cerebral metabolism was studied by intraventricular injection of L-glutamic acid into mice, when 150 mg produced convulsions or only incoordinated grooming or circling of the cage (Crawford, 1963). 2% intra-arterial sodium glutamate increased epileptic fits and intracisternal L-glutamic acid caused tonic-clonic convulsions in animals and man. High parenteral dosage of L-glutamic acid caused EEG changes only in dogs with previous cerebral damage and no rise was detected in the CSF level of glutamate (Herbst et al., 1966). L-glutamic acid is oxidized by the brain to alphaketoglutaric acid, NH3 and later CO2 and H2O and is the only amino acid that on its own can maintain brain slice respiration (Weil-Malherbe, 1936). Decarboxylation to gamma-aminobutyric acid is significant in the mammalian brain (Roberts & Frankel, 1951; Perrault & Dry, 1961). Rat An experiment was conducted feeding 45 male and 45 female rats with 1 g/kg or 250 mg/kg MSG daily from day 1 to 90 days of age at which time the animals were killed. Comparable control group received laboratory chow only over the same period of time. General clinical observations, body weights, haematologic parameters and other clinical chemical measurements were within the normal. At autopsy, organ weights were within the normal range. Histochemical and ultrastructural studies of hypothalamus and median eminence showed no evidence of repair or replacement of neuronal cells by elements of glial or ependymal cells (Golberg, 1973). Guinea-pig A single subcutaneous injection of 1 g/kg MSG was given to two or three-day old guinea-pigs. Six animals were killed three hours after treatment and the hypothalamic area investigated by histochemical and histological techniques. No clear effect of MSG was discernible. With a dose of 4 g/kg an increase in glial cells, vacualization of cell in the arcuate nuclei and some evidence of cell necrosis was observed. The severity of the lesions was in no way comparable to the effects seen in the hypothalami of mice treated with the same doses (Golberg, 1973). Monkey In a follow-up experiment, doses of 1 g or 250 mg/kg MSG daily for 30 days were administered orally to two groups of three infant rhesus monkeys starting at one day after birth. General clinical observations over a period of 30 days revealed normal growth, development and activity. No changes were observed also in the levels of haemoglobin haematocrit, RBC, WBC count or reticulocytes. The levels of glucose, urea nitrogen, serum potassium, calcium and sodium were within the normal range. At autopsy, complete histological, histochemical and ultrastructural investigations of the entire arcuate nuclei and median eminence region failed to reveal any necrotic or damaged neurons (Golberg, 1973). Acute toxicity LD50 Animal Route (mg/kg bw) References Mouse i.p. 6900 Yanagisawa et al., 1961 p.o. 12 961 Izeki, 1964 p.o. 16200 (14 200-18 400) Ichimura & Kirimura, 1968 p.o. 19 200 (22 840- 16130) Pinto-Scognamiglio et al., 1972 Mouse i.v. 30 000 Ajinomoto Co., 1970 Rat p.o. 19 900 (L MSG) International Minerals & Chem. Corp., 1969 p.o. 10 000 (DL MSG) International Minerals & Chem. Corp., 1969 p.o. > 30 000 (L-GA) International Minerals & Chem. Corp., 1969 p.o. 16 600 (18 900-14 500) Pinto-Scognamiglio et al., 1972 Guinea-pig i.p. 15 000 Ajinomoto Co., 1970 Rabbit p.o. > 2300 (L-GA) International Minerals & Chem. Corp., 1969 Cat s.c. 8000 Ajinomoto Co., 1970 Mouse Mice aged two to nine days were killed 1-48 hours after single subcutaneous injection of monosodium glutamate at doses from 0.5-4 g/kg lesions seen in the preoptic and arcuate nuclei of the hypothalamic region on the roof and floor of the third ventricle and in scattered neurons in the nuclei tuberales. No pituitary lesions were seen but sub-commissural and subfornical organs exhibited intra- cellular oedema and neuronal necrosis. Adult mice given subcutaneously 5-7 g/kg monosodium L-glutamate showed similar lesions (Olney, 1969b). After a single subcutaneous injection of monosodium glutamate at 4 g/kg into neonatal mice aged 9-10 days, the animals were killed from 30 minutes to 48 hours. The retinas showed an acute lesion on electron microscopy with swelling dendrites and early neuronal changes leading to necrosis followed by phagocytosis (Olney, 1969a). Sixty-five neonatal mice aged 10-12 days received single oral loads of monosodium glutamate at 0.5, 0.75, 1.0 and 2.0 g/kg bw by gavage. Ten were controls and 54 mice received other amounts. After three to six hours all treated animals were killed by perfusion. Brain damage as evidenced by necrotic neurons was evident in arcuate nuclei of 51 animals. 52% at 0.5 g/kg, 81% at 0.75 g/kg, 100% at 1 g/kg and 100% at 2 g/kg. The lesions were identical both by light and electron microscopy to s.c. produced lesions. No lesions were seen at 0.25 g/kg. The number of necrotic neurons rose approximately with dose. Four animals tested with glutamic acid also developed the same lesions at 1 g/kg bw. The effect was additive with aspartate (Olney, 1970b). High s.c. doses (4 or 1 g/kg of MSG caused hypothalamic changes in 60% and 42% respectively of treated five to seven day old mice. Oral administration (1 or 4 g/kg) of a 4% aqueous solution elicited a predominantly glial reaction to 26-28% of the mice. The remainder were unaffected. Four monkeys receiving 4 g/kg (4% solution) s.c. or orally at four days of age showed no difference in appearance of the arcuate, median eminence or ependymal cells when compared with controls (Abraham et al., 1971). Six, nine to ten day old mice, dosed orally with 10% monosodium glutamate (2 g/kg), showed characteristic brain lesions (Geil, 1970). Monosodium (i.p., 3.2 g/kg bw) glutamate caused reversible blockage of beta wave in the electroretinogram in immature mice and rats indicating retinotoxicity (Potts et al., 1960). The timing of treatment of mice was quite critical. After 10-11 days postnatal age it was difficult to produce significant lesions of the retina (Olney, 1969a). A study of the glutamate metabolizing enzymes of the retina of the glutamate treated rat indicated a decrease in glutaminase activity, an increase in glutamic aspartate transaminase, and no change in glutamyl synthetase and glutamotransference. The effects appear to be due to repression and induction of enzyme synthesis (Freedman & Potts, 1962; Freedman & Potts, 1963). Glutamate uptake by retina, brain and plasma decreases with age and is slower at 12 days when compared with five day old animals (Freedman & Potts, 1963). Degeneration of neonatal mouse retina has been reported following parenteral administration of MSG (10 s.c. injections 2.2-4.2 g/kg, 1-10 days after birth) (Cohen, 1967). S.c. injection of L-monosodium glutamate at 4-8 g/kg into mice caused retinal damage with ganglion cell necrosis within a few hours. In very young animals there was extensive damage to the inner layers (Lucas & Newhouse, 1957). Infant litter mates of Swiss-Webster mice were divided into two groups. The experimental group received single daily subcutaneous injections of MSG for 10 consecutive days. The control litter mate group received injections of 0.9% saline. All injections were of 0.02 ml volume. The dose of first MSG injection, started 24 hours after birth, was 2 g/kg bw. Subsequent daily doses were 2.25, 2.5-4.25 g/kg through the tenth day. When the surviving MSG treated and control mice attained 20-28 g bw, they were subjected to a battery of behavioural and pharmacological tests. The study period lasted until 50 days after birth. There were no significant or observable differences in response to behavioural tests or to selected drugs (Prabhu & Oester, 1971). Monosodium glutamate, monopotassium glutamate, sodium chloride and sodium gluconate at 1 g/kg in a 10% w/v solution (and comparable volumes of distilled water) were administered orally and subcutaneously to mice and rats at three or 12 days of age and to dogs at three or 35 days of age and the animals were killed within 24 hours of dosage. Examination of the eyes and of the preoptic and arcuate nuclei of the hypothalamus by two pathologists revealed no dose- related histomorphological effects in any of the test groups at either of the two ages selected to correspond to the periods before and at the beginning of solid food intake (Oser et al., 1971). Seventy-five infant Swiss albino mice (CD-l) (three to 10 days old) were given single subcutaneous injection of MSG in concentrations equal to 2 and 4 g/kg (0.1 ml in distilled water). Another group of 50 adult mice (CD-1) were injected either subcutaneously or intraperitoneally with MSG in doses varying from 6 to 10 g/kg (1 ml volume). Controls were injected with sodium chloride. Mice were sacrificed between three and 72 hours post-dosing. Brain tissue was examined by light and electron microscopy. 95% of the animals injected with MSG developed brain lesions in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus. Lesions involved primarily microglial cells with no effect to the perikarya of neurons. Distal neuronal processes were only slightly affected (Arees & Mayer, 1971). Thirteen neonate mice (CF # I-JCL) received a single subcutaneous injection of 1 g/kg of MSG a day two and four after birth. Brains were removed one, three, six and 24 hours post-injection and examined by light microscopy. The common finding after three and six hours was necrosis of the neural element in the region of the hippocampus and hypothalamus. When pregnant mice of the same strain were injected subcutaneously with 5 mg/g on day 17 and 18 of pregnancy, examination of fetal brain three, six and 24 hours after treatment of mother showed cellular necrosis in both the ventromedial and arcuate nuclei (Murakami & Inouye, 1971). Groups each of five three-day old mice C57BL/J6 strain, were given either a single intragastric or single subcutaneous dose of monosodium glutamate, sodium chloride, sodium gluconate, potassium glutamate (all 10% solutions, 10 ml/kg bw) or water. Groups of 12-day-old mice were also treated in a similar manner. All animals were killed 24 hours after dosing. Microscopic examination of the brain, particularly the ventral hypothalamus did not show any neuronal necrosis of the hypothalamic arcuate nuclei (Oser et al., 1973). Groups each of 10 10-day-old Webster Swiss albino mice were given a single subcutaneous dose of one of 24 compounds structurally related to MSG. The dose level was either 12 or 24 m moles/kg bw. Five hours post-dosing brains and retinas were processed for light or electron microscopy. Roughly quantifying the pathological reaction in the infant hypothalamus was used as a method for comparing the neurotoxic potency of the test compounds. Except for L-cysteine, all neurotoxic compounds were acidic amino acids known to excite neurones. The most potent neurotoxic compounds were those known to be powerful neuroexcitants (N-methyl DL-aspartic and DL-homocysteic acids) (Olney et al., 1971). Groups each of 15 Webster Swiss albino mice 10-12 days old were given a single oral dose of MSG at a level of 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1.0 and 2.0 g/kg. Groups of two and four mice of the same age were given a single oral dose of either L-glutamic acid, monosodium-L-aspartate, L-glutamate-L-aspartate, monosodium glutamate, NaCl, L-glycine, L-serine, L-alanine, L-Leucine, D-L methionine, L-phenylalanine, L-proline, L-lysine, L-arginine, L-cysteine at a dose level of 3 g/kg. The post-dosing animals were sacrificed and brains examined by either light or electron microscopy. The severity of brain damage was estimated by quantifying the pathological changes in the hypothalamus. 1 g/kg dose of glutamic acid destroyed approximately the same number of hypothalamic neurones as a comparable dose of MSG. Of the amino acids tested, only aspartate and cysteine produced hypothalamic damage. These amino acids caused both retinal and hypothalamic lesions similar to those found after treatment with MSG (Olney & Ho, 1970). MSG was injected subcutaneously into four-day-old mice at a dose level of 2 g/µg bw, administered as an aqueous solution. Mice were sacrificed at 7.5, 15 and 30 minutes and one, three, six, nine, 12 and 18 hours post-injection. Brains were disected and glutamate levels determined in arcuate nucleus, ventromedial hypothalamus and lateral thalamus. Blood levels of glutamate were also determined at these time intervals. Glutamate levels in the arcuate nucleus reached a maximum three hours post-dosing (111 m mole/kg dry weight). The highest levels were reached in the ventromedial hypothalamus and lateral thalamus about nine hours post-dosing and were about 42 m mole/kg dry weight. Glutamate levels in the three brain regions returned to control values (about 25 m mole/kg dry weight) about 12-15 hours post-dosing. Blood glutamate levels reached a maximum (40 mM) within 15 minutes from a basal level of 0.12 mM but returned to baseline values (below 1 mM) in the one to three hour interval after injection (Perez & Olney, 1972). Rat Groups each of 20 10-day-old rats, Charles River strain, (10 male, 10 female) were dosed orally with 0.2 ml of either strained baby food containing no monosodium glutamate, strained baby food containing monosodium glutamate up to 0.4%, or strained baby food containing monosodium glutamate equal to a dosage level of 0.5 g/kg, additional to that found in normal commercially distributed baby food (390 mg per jar). The rats were mated and half of the offspring were removed from parental females, and sacrificed after five hours. Histological studies were made of brain in the area of the hypothalamus. at the roof and the floor of the third ventricle. The remaining rats were returned to parental females and allowed to grow to maturity (90 days post-weaning), then sacrificed and histological studies made of the brain. No lesions were observed in the brain of animals sacrificed at either five hours post-treatment or after reaching maturity. Animals which were reared to maturity showed normal growth and food consumption (Geil, 1970). Male and female rats of the Wistar strain, three to four days old, received a single subcutaneous injection of MSG. The total dose was 4 g/kg bw in a volume of 0.1 ml. Control rats were injected with an equal volume of saline. In experiments concerned with the acute effects of MSG on the brain the infant rats were killed three hours after the injection of MSG. Light microscopic and electron microscopic examination failed to reveal any effects upon the lateral pre-optic nucleus, arcuate nucleus, and median eminence. To determine long-range effects of MSG, uniform litters (eight pups per mother) were kept in an environmentally controlled room until weaned. The experiment was terminated at 68 days post-treatment for males and 88 days post- treatment for females. In the MSG-treated females the relative ovarian weights were significantly less than in the controls (29.3 ± 1.4 mg/100 g versus 34.7 ± 0.9 P < 0.1), but otherwise there were no significant differences in weight of reproductive organs (ovaries, testes, seminal vesicles, prostate). The adult MSG-treated females cycled normally and were capable of mating and producing normal litters (Adamo & Ratner, 1970). In a letter to Science it is pointed out that Adamo & Ratner injected a 40% solution of MSG instead of the 10% solution used by Olney et al. (Olney, 1971). Weanling Sprague-Dawley male rats were given MSG intraperitoneally 20 mmol = 3.4 g/kg bw. Marked somnolence was observed within five to 20 minutes. About 1/3 to 1/2 of the animals salivated copiously and had myocolonic jerking about one hour after injection, sometimes followed by vigorous running about the cage and sterotyped biting (Bhagavan et al., 1971). At birth rat brain glutamate is about 4 mM and increases over a period of 20 days to the adult value of approximately 10 mM. When a 4 g/kg dose was given intragastrically, convulsions were seldom observed and then only after 90 minutes. MSG given intraperitoneally always caused convulsions with 2 g/kg. When young rats were given large doses of MSG, monosodium aspartate or glycine at 4 g/kg, the glutamine level was increased significantly in the brain by all amino acids, but only monosodium glutamate and aspartate caused convulsions. D-glutamate (4 g/kg) which is not deaminated by the rat also causes convulsions. These results suggest that the convulsions of MSG are not due to liberated ammonia but rather to the amino acid anions. At 4 g/kg MSG gave rise to serum concentrations of glutamate of about 70 mM, strongly suggesting osmotic problems (Mushahwar & Koeppe, 1971). Fifty weanling rats (FDRL strain), three days of age, were divided into five groups. Each group was subdivided and the test animals given either a single intragastric or single subcutaneous dose of monosodium glutamate, sodium chloride, sodium gluconate, potassium glutamate (all 10% solutions, 10 ml/kg bw), or water. Another group of 12-day-old rats were treated in a similar manner. All animals in these groups were killed 24 hours after dosing. In addition, another group of 60 rats (three days of age) was divided into groups and treated with the same test compounds at the same dose levels. Half of each group was sacrificed at six hours and the other half at 24 hours after dosing. Microscopic examination of brain, particularly the ventral hypothalamus did not show neuronal necrosis of the hypothalamic arcuate nuclei, except in one rat dosed with monosodium glutamate 1 g/kg at three days of age, and killed 24 hours later showed an area in the median eminence which contained cells with slight nuclear pyknosis and prominent vacuolation (Oser et al., 1973). MSG was administered at dose levels of 1 g/µg to infant mice and 2 and 4 g/µg to infant rats. All the animals developed lesions in the arcuate area of the hypothalamus and median eminence. No evidence of cellular pathology was noted in controls (Burde et al., 1971). Eight litter mates from each of 10 pregnant Holtzmann rats were divided into four groups consisting of two litters from each mother. Litters of these groups received water (control) and MSG solution (1.25, 2.5 and 5 g/kg) by stomach tube daily from day 5 to 10 of age. At day 21 rats were placed in separate cages and at three months of age were subjected to three different behavioural situations, namely, spontaneous motor activity, T-maze and fixed-ratio food reinforcement. Rats of the high dose level showed less spontaneous motor activity and deficiency in discrimination learning in the T-maze study. However, learning of a fixed ratio food reinforcement schedule was not affected* (Pradham & Lynch, 1972). Rabbit I.v. injection of sodium L-glutamate produced a prolonged increase in the amino-nitrogen content of blood and prolonged urinary excretion in rabbits (Yamamura, 1960). I.v. injection of large doses of glutamic acid in rabbits, caused ECG changes that could be interpreted as symptoms of myocardial lesions. Arterial hypertension induced by glutamic acid preparations was demonstrated to be of central origin. Studies with isolated heart showed that large doses of glutamic acid slowed heart action, increased systolic amplitude and constricted coronary vessels. Very large doses stopped cardiac action (Mazurowa et al., 1962). Dog Intravenous casein hydrolysate or synthetic amino acid mixture caused nausea and vomiting in dogs (Madden et al., 1944). Groups of three dogs at three days or 35 days of age received subcutaneously or orally a single acute dose (1 g/kg) monosodium glutamate, monopotassium glutamate, sodium chloride, sodium gluconate or distilled water; and were sacrificed three hours and 24 hours after treatment. Preliminary light microscopic studies of the large midbrain area showed similar non-specific scattered tissue changes in all treated groups (Oser et al., 1971). Groups each of six pups, three to four days of age were dosed either orally or subcutaneously with monosodium glutamate, sodium chloride, sodium gluconate, potassium glutamate (1 g/kg bw) or water. Pups were sacrificed at either three hours, 24 hours or 52 weeks after dosing. Other groups of dogs 35 days old received a single dose of test material, and were sacrificed at either four hours or 24 hours * After three months the treated animals had a weight gain which was less than controls, the gain being less in the 5 g/kg dose group. post-dosing. Body weights of dogs which were dosed once at three days of age and followed for a year, showed no evidence of effect of any treatment. Femur weight, as well as weight of pituitary gland, ovaries, uterus and mammary glands were similar to controls. Gross and microscopic examination of these tissues failed to reveal any abnormalities. Extensive microscopic examination of brain tissue of all test animals did not show any treatment-related changes (Oser et al., 1973). Monkey A newborn (eight hours old) rhesus infant, probably somewhat premature was given subcutaneously 2.7 g/kg bw monosodium glutamate. After three hours (no abnormal behaviour noted) the monkey was killed and the brain perfused in situ for 20 minutes. A lesion was seen in the periventricular arcuate region of the hypothalamus identical to those seen in mice given similar treatment. Electron microscopic pathological changes were seen in dendrites and neuron cells but not in glia or vascular components (Olney & Sharpe, 1969). Monkeys, four days old, received a single dose of monosodium glutamate (4 g/kg in phosphate buffer), either subcutaneously or orally. Animals receiving subcutaneous injections were sacrificed at three, 24 and 72 hours, the one receiving an oral dose at 24 hours. No brain lesions were observed (Abraham et al., 1971). Three infant monkeys, five days of age, received MSG by stomach tubes at a dose of 2 g/kg. Two infant monkeys at 10 days of age, two at 20 days of age and two at 40 days of age received the same treatment. Two animals at 80 days of age received 4 g/kg. The animals were observed for four hours after dosing and then sacrificed. After a period of fixation a block of tissue was removed from each brain to include the hypothalamus. Serial sections, 10 mm thick were made in the horizontal plane and examined by light microscopy. No changes were observed in the hypothalamus of the monkeys that were considered to be associated with the administration of MSG. One control monkey was included in each group (Huntington, 1971). Two infant monkeys at two days of age were given monosodium glutamate by intragastric incubation at the dose level of 2 g/kg and two infant monkeys of the same ages were used as control. The brains were studied by electron microscopy. No morphological differences were observed between treated and control monkeys. The changes observed appeared to be due to fixation artifact and cannot be attributed to monosodium glutamate (Huntington, 1971). A group of three to four day old cynomolgus monkeys received either subcutaneously or orally a single dose of monosodium glutamate, sodium chloride, sodium gluconate, potassium glutamate (1 g/kg bw) and were sacrificed three or 24 hours post-dosing. Another group of monkeys (three to four days old) received orally either 4 g/kg monosodium glutamate or sodium chloride, and were sacrificed at three, six and 24 hours post-dosing (three and 24 hours in case of sodium chloride dosed monkeys). Detailed microscopic examination of the hypothalamus did not show any features of monosodium glutamate induced necrosis or any differences between any of the groups. Examination of the eyes did not reveal any monosodium glutamate effect. Glutamate and glutamine blood levels of monkeys showed considerable variation in individual values between oral and subcutaneous doses. Subcutaneous dosing resulted in values of an order of magnitude higher than those observed by oral dosing (Oser et al., 1973). Monosodium glutamate was administered to six pregnant rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) at a daily dosage equivalent to 4 g/kg bw during the last third of pregnancy. Four pregnant monkeys not receiving treatment were used as controls. Body weight and condition was unaffected throughout the gestation period. The duration of gestation was within accepted range (156-178 days). There were no cases of delayed parturition or dystocia. Nursing, suckling and behavioural patterns were normal except for one monkey which killed its infant at birth. Birth weight of neonates were within the normal range. Infants, when removed from mothers, showed distress but no signs of abnormal behaviour. About four hours after birth, infant monkeys were sacrificed. The hypothalamus region and related structure of the brain was examined by light microscopy. No abnormalities were observed (Heywood et al., 1972a). Monosodium glutamate was administered by intragastric intubation at dosage levels of 2 g/kg to two monkeys aged two days. Two monkeys of similar age were used for control. Four hours after dosing, animals were sacrificed. Examination of the hypothalamus (bordered rostrally by the optic chiasma and caudally by the pons), by light and electron microscopy, did not show changes with the administration of the test compound. Changes observed by electron microscopy occurred as frequently in control animals as in test animals, and appear to be due to fixation artifacts (Heywood et al., 1972b). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) aged between four and 80 days were divided into groups by age. Each group contained three test and one control animal. Dosage was 2 g/kg for animals up to 44 days of age and 4 g/kg for animals up to 80 days of age. The animals were observed for four hours and then sacrificed. Immediately prior to dosing and prior to sacrifice, serum and plasma samples were obtained for measurement of SGPT, SGOT and plasma glutamic acid. At sacrifice liver samples were obtained for measurement of glutamic pyruvic transaminase and glutamic oxalacetic transaminase. SGPT and SGOT values did not show any significant increase over test period. Plasma glutamic acid was within the normal range. Liver GPT and GOT were within the normal range. Examination of hypothalamus region and associated structures, by light microscopy, did not reveal any compound-related effects (Heywood et al., 1971). Sixteen infant monkeys (M. mulatta or M. irus) were fasted for four hours before receiving by stomach tube a single dose of 50% solution of monosodium glutamate, equivalent to a dose of 1, 2 or 4 g/kg bw. Control animals received distilled water. At six hours post-dosing the animals were sacrificed and the brains perfused for examination by light and electron microscopy; No morphological differences were observed in the hypothalamic regions of treated and control monkeys. Inadequately fixed tissue had the same appearance as that of previously reported brain lesion in a newborn monkey (Reynolds et al., 1971). Monosodium glutamate was administered into the circulation of primate fetuses (Macaca species), via the umbilical vein, at dose levels approximately 4 g/kg bw. A total of seven animals were treated. At time periods of two to six hours post-dosing the fetus was delivered by Caesarean section and the brain fixed for microscopic examination. The hypothalamic areas of the brain from all seven fetuses were found to be completely normal. There was no evidence of pyknotic nuclei, tissue oedema or neuronal loss in the arcuate region (Reynolds & Lemkey-Johnston, 1973). Ten infant squirrel monkeys were fed either a 0%, 4.8%, 9.1% and 17% (based on dry weight) MSG formula diet for nine weeks. Three of the test monkeys died. Two died of effects not related to MSG. The third on the 17% diet developed convulsive seizures. However, the other two animals in this group were unaffected. Clinical observations were made daily, and at the end of the test period, the monkeys were sacrificed and the major organs examined microscopically. Sections of the retina and hypothalamus were examined by electron microscopy. No hypothalamic or retinal lesions were observed. In another study an infant cynomologus monkey and an infant brush monkey were fed 9.1% MSG formula for one year. Daily observations revealed no behavioural abnormalities. Their weight gains, ERG, EEG and plasma amino acid were similar to controls not consuming MSG. No evidence of gross obesity developed (Wen et al., 1973). Groups of three infant monkeys were dosed with a mixture of water and skim milk containing either added NaCl or MSG, on an equivalent mole/kg basis. Administration was via nasogastric tube. Other groups were injected subcutaneously with either a 25% aqueous solution of MSG or a 10% solution of NaCl. The doses tested ranged from 1-4 g/kg bw. All animals were sacrificed post-dosing and brains examined by combined light and electron microscopy. Infants given relatively low oral doses of MSG (1 and 2 g/kg) sustained small focal lesions confined primarily to the rostro-ventral aspect of the infundibular nucleus. Those treated with high subcutaneous doses developed lesions which spread throughout and sometimes beyond the infundibular nucleus at all doses tested, and by either route of administration, rapid necrosis of neurons (within five hours) was observed. Measurements of blood glutamate levels, suggested that the threshold for lesion formation in one week old rhesus monkeys may be in the range of 200 mg/l (Olney et al., 1972). Short-term studies Mouse Thirty-eight neonate mice were observed for nine months. Twenty received subcutaneous monosodium glutamate daily for one to 10 days after birth in doses of 2.2-4.2 g/kg. Eighteen were controls. Although treated animals remained skeletally stunted and both males and females gained more weight than controls from 30 to 150 days yet treated animals consumed less food than controls. Test animals were lethargic, females failed to conceive but male fertility was not affected. At autopsy of test animals massive fat accumulation was seen in test mice, fatty livers, thin uteri and adenohypophysis had overall fewer cells in the adenohypophysis. Ten test neonates received a single subcutaneous injection of 3 g/kg monosodium glutamate two days after birth, 13 neonates were controls. Again test animals were heavier than controls after nine months but less so than mice given repeated injection treatment. For the latter it was postulated that an endocrine disturbance would lead to skeletal stunting, adiposity and female sterility. Lesions differed from those due to gold thioglucose or bipiperidyl mustard which affect the ventro-medial nucleus and cause hyperphagia (Olney, 1969b). Rat Natural monosodium L-glutamate, synthetic monosodium L-glutamate, and synthetic monosodium D-glutamate in amounts of 20, 200 and 2000 mg/kg bw were given orally to groups of five male rats each once a day for a period of 90 days. No effects on body weight, growth, volume and weight of cerebrum, cerebellum, heart, stomach, liver, spleen and kidneys in comparison with the control group were observed. No histological changes in internal organs were found by macroscopic and microscopic examination (Hara et al., 1962). Male albino rats (Sprague-Dawley) were allowed water ad libitum and fed ground laboratory chow, 24% protein content. High levels of single amino acids in the diet of rats beginning at 21 days produced a decreased food intake and a severe growth depression. These effects were dependent upon the kind and the concentration of amino acid supplemented. L-methionine caused the most severe growth depression while L-phenylalanine, L-tryptophan and L-cystine were also severely toxic. Less toxic were L-histidine, L-lysine and L-tyrosine. All other amino acids tested including glutamic acid had only a slight effect on growth or none at all. Growth depression was attributed both to a depressed food intake and to a specific toxic effect of the amino acid. A direct correlation was found between the toxicity of any dietary amino acid and its concentration in the blood (Daniel & Waisman, 1968). Nine groups of 20 rats were given 0.5% and 6% of calcium glutamate in their diet. No effect was noted on maze learning or recovery from ECT shock (Porter & Griffin, 1950). Two groups of 14 rats received 200 mg L-monosodium glutamate per animal for 35 days. No difference in their learning ability for maze trials was noted (Stellar & McElroy, 1948). Eight male rats fed 5% dietary DL-glutamic acid in a low protein diet (6% protein) showed little or no depression of growth, when compared to low protein controls. There was a 50% increase in the free glutamic acid in the plasma (Sauberlich, 1961). Long-term studies Mouse One control group of 200 male mice and six test groups of 100 male mice received 0%, 1% or 4% in their diet of either L-glutamic acid, monosodium L-glutamate or DL-monosodium glutamate. No malignant tumours appeared after two years that could be related to the administration of test material. Growth and haematology were normal, histopathology showed no abnormalities in the test animals (Little, 1953a). Rat Groups of 75 male and 75 female rats received for two years dietary levels of 0, 0.1% or 0.4% either monosodium L-glutamate, monosodium DL-glutamate or L-glutamic acid respectively. No adverse effects were noted on body weight, growth, food intake, haematology, general behaviour, survival rate, gross and histopathology or tumour incidence (Little, 1953b). OBSERVATIONS IN MAN Pharmacological effects were studied in 56 normal subjects (30 male and 26 female) after oral administration of L-glutamate on an empty stomach. Symptoms occurred in all but one subject. Three categories of symptoms could be elicited by monosodium glutamate: burning, facial and chest pressure. Headaches were a consistent complaint of a minority of individuals. Oral administration of monosodium glutamate caused symptoms after 15-25 minutes. The oral threshold range for minimum symptoms in 36 subjects was from 1.5-12 g (Schaumburg et al., 1969). Similar effects were obtained by 3-5 g of monopotassium glutamate, L-glutamic acid and DL-glutamic acid but no effects were seen with monosodium D-glutamate or other L-aminoacids. Thirteen subjects received i.v. 25-125 mg sodium glutamate with symptoms occurring within 20 seconds. The burning sensation is due to a peripheral mechanism and no genetic predisposition was noted (Schaumburg et al., 1969). A survey was made of 912 Japanese individuals to determine if any of these symptoms were noted after eating a prepared Oriental Type Noodle, containing 0.61-1.36 g monosodium glutamate/serving. In no case were any of the characteristic symptoms reported (Ichimura et al., 1970a). In another study, the effect of monosodium glutamate on 61 healthy men was determined by the double blind method. The doses of monosodium glutamate administered were 2.2 g, 4.4 g or 8.7 g. Intake was either on a non-empty stomach (30 minutes after meal) or an empty stomach (overnight fast). In experiments on the non-empty stomach conditions the number of persons showing some symptoms were the same for the placebo and the others. In the case of the empty stomach conditions a number of the test subjects on the highest level of monosodium glutamate experienced two of the typical symptoms at the same time. No individual experienced three of the symptoms. The effect of monosodium glutamate intake (2.2 g, 4.4 g or 8.7 g) on changes in blood pressure, pulse rate, ECG, and sodium and glutamate levels in blood, was measured in five persons who had not experienced any symptoms, and nine who had experienced some symptoms. There were no differences in increase in glutamic acid in the blood in either group. Sodium content of the blood and all other parameters measured showed no changes in either group (Ichimura et al., 1970b). The occurrence of nausea and vomiting following the i.v. administration of various preparations in a series of 57 human subjects was found to parallel the free glutamic acid content of the mixture. There was a direct relationship between free serum glutamic acid and the occurrence of toxic effects, following i.v. administration. When the serum glutamic acid reached 12 to 15 mg/100 ml, nausea and vomiting occurred in half the subjects. Other amino acids appear to potentiate the effect (Levey et al., 1949). Intravenous glutamic acid (100 mg/kg) produces vomiting (Madden et al., 1944). Arginine glutamate may be used in the treatment of ammonia intoxication. It is given by intravenous infusion in doses of 25 to 50 g every eight hours for three to five days in dextrose and infused at a rate of not more than 25 g of arginine glutamate over one to two hours. More rapid infusions may cause vomiting (Martindale, 1967). Single and double blind studies were done with single oral doses of monosodium glutamate in human male volunteers on a fasting stomach (18 hours after last meal). Ninety-eight received 5 g of monosodium glutamate in single blind studies, six received 8 g and five received 12 g in double blind studies. Physical examinations were done on all subjects. Complaints were registered in all groups ranging from 23-80%. There was a low incidence of most complaints except for lightheadedness and tightness in the face. No subject reported or was observed to have experienced the complete triad of symptoms as described in the original Chinese-Restaurant syndrome (Kwok's disease). In the double blind studies where clinical chemistry, blood pressure and pulse were measured in addition to clinical examination, no significant differences between monosodium glutamate and sodium chloride were detected (Rosenblum et al., 1971), Monosodium glutamate has been used in the treatment of mentally retarded children in doses up to 48 g daily but on average 10-15 g was given. One hundred and fifty children aged 4-15 years were treated with glutamic acid for six months and compared with 50 controls. There was a rise in verbal intelligence quotient but was not statistically significant. 64% showed improvement of behavioural traits (Zimmerman & Burgemeister, 1959). Seventeen patients received up to 15 g monosodium glutamate three times a day but showed a raised blood level for 12 hours only. No effect on BMR, EEG, ECG, BP, heart rate, respiration rate, temperature and weight was noted over 11 months (Himwich et al., 1954a). Fifteen grams then 30 g monosodium glutamate were given per dose for one week each, followed by 48 g for 12 weeks to 53 patients without any effect on basal plasma levels of glutamic acid (Himwich et al., 1954b). DL-glutamic acid HCl was given in doses of 12, 16 and 20 g to eight patients with petit mal and psychomotor epilepsy without adverse effects (Price et al., 1943). Five episodes of hepatic coma in three patients were treated with i.v. 23 g of monosodium glutamate with improvement (Walshe, 1953). Ten to 12 g of L-glutamic acid given to epileptics and mental defectives appeared to improve nine out of 20 cases (Waelsch, 1949). In a study with double blind and crossover techniques, MSG was administered to 24 healthy subjects aged 18 to 24 years, MSG was given "at lunch time in doses of 3 g/subject in 150 ml of beef broth". No differences in subjective symptoms were observed between the subjects given MSG and those who had normal beef broth. Nobody, either in the control or in the MSG group, experienced the burning feeling which is typical of the Chinese restaurant syndrome (Morselli & Garattini, 1970). Six women with well established lactation patterns were fasted overnight and received a single oral dose of 6 g monosodium glutamate in water and liquid diet formulation and four controls received lactose. Milk samples were obtained one, two, three, four, six and 12 hours after administration. Blood samples were taken 0, 30, 60, 120 and 180 minutes after administration of MSG in water, and 0, 60, 90, 150 and 210 minutes after administration of MSG in a liquid diet formulation or the lactose. Small increases of plasma glutamate aspartate and alanine levels were noted. Little change was observed in breast milk amino acid levels (Stegink et al., 1972). One hundred and twelve human subjects in an unselected population received doses of MSG ranging from 1 to 20 g dissolved in water. 32% of the persons tested responded at the 5 g level when challenged by a single placebo controlled exposure. The subjects showed dose-response relationship, particularly marked in the case of stiffness and tightness symptoms and somewhat less marked in the case of pressure, warmth, and tingling sensation which exhibited a clear threshold for their appearance at the 2 to 3 g level (Kenney & Tidball, 1972). Comments: Glutamic acid is a component of proteins and comprises some 20% of ingested protein. During gastrointestinal absorption transamination to alanine occurs, consequently there is only a slight rise in glutamate levels in the portal blood of animals and probably also of man. However, if the capacity of this mechanism and the further conversion of glutamate in the liver is overwhelmed, or if a glutamate is administered parenterally in large doses, it is possible to obtain high blood levels. It has been demonstrated that, at least in the rat and rhesus monkey, glutamate does not readily cross the placental barrier or the blood-brain barrier, nor is it secreted in human mother's milk. Numerous reproduction studies in mice, rats, rabbits and monkeys, revealed no deleterious effects on the offspring if tho parent generation was fed glutamate in high doses, suggesting that the fetus or young infant are not exposed to high levels of glutamate through the mother's diet. The earlier claim of teratogenic effects in the rabbit was probably not related to glutamate administration. Some investigations have demonstrated a vulnerability of the developing mouse, rat or primate CNS to high levels of glutamate in addition to other amino acids following subcutaneous administration. Neonatal mice are relatively susceptible to the development of a highly localized lesion in the arcuate nuclei of the hypothalamus with incomplete myelination. Oral administration of high doses failed to reproduce these lesions in neonatal rats, guinea-pigs or monkeys, which had been reported by previous investigators. Acute reactions reported after ingestion of glutamate are probably due to the rapid absorption of large amounts of the substance (greater than 3 g): this syndrome has been termed Kwok's disease. On the data provided it is possible to arrive at an acceptable daily intake for glutamate and its salts making allowance for the fact that glutamate is a normal constituent of protein and free glutamate also occurs in the diet. In view of the uncertainty regarding the possible susceptibility of the very early human neonate to high oral intakes of glutamate, it would be prudent to avoid adding glutamate to foods intended for infants under 12 weeks of age. Neither the fetus nor the suckling infant are at risk from maternal ingestion of glutamate. There is no reason to suspect that the potassium, sodium, calcium and ammonium salts exert effects other than those due to the respective cations. EVALUATION Estimate of acceptable daily intake for man 0-120* ag/kg bw. FURTHER WORK REQUIRED IF USE IS TO BE EXTENDED TO INFANT FOODS 1. Oral non-adverse effects levels of glutamates in neonatal animals. 2. Age correlations between neonatal experimental animals and the human infant. REFERENCES Abraham, R., Dougherty, W., Goldberg, L. & Coulston, F. (1971) Exp. Molec. Path., 15, 43 Adamo, N. J. &Ratner, A. (1970) Science, 169, 673-674 Adkins, J. S., Wertz, J. M., Boffmann, R. H. & Hove, E. L. (1967) Proc. Soc. exp. Biol. (N.Y.), 126, 500-504 Ajinomoto Co., Inc. (1970) Unpublished Report Aleksandrov, P. N., Bogdanova, V. A. & Chernukh, A. M. (1965) Farm. i. Toks., 28(6), 744 Ando, F. (1959) Osaka Shir. Dai. Ig. Z., 8, 1305 * Calculated as glutamic acid, additional to glutamic acid intake from all non-additive dietary sources. It does not apply to infants under 12 weeks of age. Arees, E. & Mayer, J. (1971) Paper presented at the 47th Annual Meeting of Amer. Assoc. Neuropath. in Puerto Rico, June Aroskar, J. P. & Berg, C. P. (1962) Arch. Biochem. Biophys., 98, 286 Berfenstam, R., Jazenburg, R. & Mellander, O. (1955) Acta Paed., 44, 348 Van den Berg, C. J., Krazalic, L. J., Mela, P. & Waelsch, H. (1969) Biochem. J., 113, 281 Berl, S. (1965) J. Biol. Chem., 240, 2047 Berry, W. T. C. (1970) Unpublished material from surveys by Dept. Health & Soc. Security, London Bessman, S. P., Magnes, J., Schwerin, P. & Waelsch, H. (1948) J. Biol. Chem., 175, 817 Bhagavan, H. N., Coursin, D. B. & Stewart, C. N. (1971) Nature, 232, 275-276 Burde, R. M., Schainker, B. & Kayes, J. (1971) Nature, 233, 58 Christensen, H. N., Streicher, J. A. & Elbinger, R. L. (1948) J. Biol. Chem., 172, 575 Cohen, A. I. (1967) Amer. J. Anat., 120, 319 Cohen, P. P. (1949) Biochem. J., 33, 1478 Crawford, J. M. (1963) J. Biol. Chem., 240, 1443 Daniel, R. G. & Waisman, H. A. (1968) Growth, 32, 255 Dickinson, J. C. & Hamilton, P. B. (1966) J. Neurochem., 13, 1179 Duggen, A. W. & Johnston, G. A. R. (1970) J. Neurochem., 17, 1205-1208 Ebert, A. G. (1970) Toxicol. Appl. Pharmacol., 17, 274 von Euler, H., Adler, E., Günther, G. & Das, N. B. (1938) Z. Physiol. Chem., 254, 61 Fernandez-Flores, E., Kline, D. A., Johnson, A. R. & Leber, B. L. (1970) J. of the AOAC, 53, 1203-1208 Francesconi, R. P. & Villee, C. A. (1968) Biochem. Bioph. Res. Comm., 31, 713 Freedman, J. K. & Potts, A. M. (1962) Invest. Ophthal., 1, 118 Freedman, J. K. & Potts, A. M. (1963) Invest. Ophthal., 2, 252 Fumiwake, E. (1957) Osaka Dai. Ig. Z., 9, 333 Furuya, H. (1967) Unpublished Report Garattini, S. (1971) Unpublished report from Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche "Mario Negri", 20157 Milan Geil, R. G. (1970) Prelim. Comm. Gerber Products Co. Genkin, A. M. & Udintsev, N. A. (1957) Trudy 20 G. N. S. Sverdl. Med. Int., 22, 92 Golberg, L. (1973) Unpublished report from the Institute of Experimental Pathology and Toxicology, Albany Medical College Gorbunova, Z. V., Yasakova, O. I. & Udintsev, N. A. (1960) Terap. Arkh., 32(8), 50 Gottschewski, G. H. M. (1967) Arzneimittel-Forsch., 17, 1100 Hanson, H. L., Burskway, M. J. & Lineweaver, H. (1960) Food Techn., 14, 320 Hara, S., Shibuya, T., Nakakawaji, K., Kyu, M., Nakamura, Y., Hoshikawa, H., Takeuchi, T., Iwao, T. & Ino, H. (1962) Tokyo Ikadaigaku Zasshi, J. Tokyo Med. Coll., 20(1), 69 Hashimoto, S., Ichimura, M. & Kirimura, J. (1970) Unpublished report of Central Research Lab., Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Hazelton Laboratories (1966) Report to International Mineral & Chemical Corporation dated 3 November 1966 Hazelton Laboratories (1969a) Addendum to report of 1966 dated 18 July 1969 Hazelton Laboratories (1969b) Addendum to report of 1966 dated 10 December 1969 Hepburn, F. N. & Bradley, W. B. (1964) J. Nutr., 84, 305 Hepburn, F. N., Calhoun, W. K. & Bradley, W. B. (1960) J. Nutr., 72, 163 Herbst, A., Wiechert, P. & Hennecke, H. (1966) Experientia, 22(11), 718 Heywood, R., Palmer, A. K., Newman, A. J., Barry, D. H. & Edwards, F. P. (1972b) Unpublished report to Central Research Lab., Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Heywood, R., Palmer, A. K. & Hague, P. H. (1971) Unpublished report submitted to Central Research Lab., Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Heywood, R., Palmer, A. K. & Newman, A. J. (1972a) Unpublished report to Central Research Lab., Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Himwich, H. E., Wolff, K., Hunsicker, A. L. & Himwich, W. A. (1954a) Appl. Physiol., 7, 40 Himwich, W. A. (1954) Science, 120, 351 Himwich, W. A., Petersen, I. M. & Graves, J. P. (1954b) Appl. Physiol., 7, 196 Huntington Research Center, 18 October 1971, Unpublished report to WHO Hutchinson, J. H. & Labby, D. H. (1965) Amer. J. Dig. Dis., 10, 814 Ichimura, M., Ohara, Y., Hashimoto, S., Fujimoto, T., Hasegawa, Y. & Kirimura, J. (1970c) Unpublished report of Central Research Laboratories, Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Ichimura, M., Tanaka, M., Tomita, K., Kirimura, J. & Ishizaki, T. (1970b) Unpublished report of Central Research Laboratories, Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Ichimura, M., Tanaka, M., Tomita, K., Kirimura, J. & Ishizaki, T. (1970a) Unpublished report of Central Research Laboratories, Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Ichimura, M. & Kirimura, J. (1968) Unpublished report of Central Research Laboratories, Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Industrial Bio-Test (1973) Unpublished report submitted to W. Underwood Company Inoue, T. (1960) Yakugaku Zasshi, 80, 718-720 Int. Mineral and Chemical Corporation (1969) Unpublished report Izeki, T. (1964) Report of the Osaka Municipal Hygienic Laboratory, 23, 82 Johnson, J. L. & Aprison, M. H. (1970) Brain Research, 24, 285-292 Johnson, J. L. & Aprison, M. H. (1971) Brain Research, 26, 141-148 Kamin, H. & Handler, P. (1950) J. Biol. Chem., 188, 193 Kenney, R. A. & Tidball (1972) Am. J. Clin. Nutr., 25, 140 Kerr, G. R. & Waisman, H. A. (1967) In: Amino acid Metabolism and Genetic Variation, Nyhan, W. L. (ed.), New York, McGraw-Hill Book Co. Khera, K. S., Whitta, L. L. & Nera, E. A. (1970) Unpublished results of Research Lab., Food & Drug Directorate, Ottawa, Canada Krnjevic, K. (1970) Nature, 228, 119-124 Lajtha, A., Berl, S. & Waelsch, H. (1959) J. Neurochem., 3, 322 Levey, S., Harroun, J. E. & Smyth, C. J. (1949) J. Lab. Clin. Med., 34, 1238 Levi, G. & Morisi, G. (1971) Brain Research, 26, 131-140 Little, A. D. (1953a) Report to International Mineral & Chemical Corporation dated 13 January 1953 Little, A. D. (1953b) Report to International Mineral & Chemical Corporation dated 15 March 1953 Lucas, D. R. & Newhouse, J. P. (1957) Amer. Med. Ass. Arch. Ophthalm., 58, 193 Madden, S. C., Woods, R. R., Skull, F. W. & Whipple, G. H. (1944) J. exp. Med., 79, 607 Maeda, S., Eguchi, S. & Sasaki, H. (1958) J. Home Econ., 9, 163 Maeda, S., Eguchi, S. & Sasaki, H. (1961) J. Home Econ., 12, 105 Marcus, R. & Reaven, G. (1967) Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med., 124, 970 Martindale, W. (1967) Extra Pharmacopoeia, 25th ed. Matthews, D. M. & Wiseman, G. (1953) J. Physiol., 120, 55 Mazurowa, A., Mrozikiewicz, A. & Wrocinski, T. (1962) Acta Physiol. Polonica, 13, 797 McColl, J. D., Globus, M. & Robinson, S. (1965) Canad. J. Phys. Pharm., 43, 69 McLaughlan, J. M., Noel, F. J., Botting, H. G. & Knipfel, J. E. (1970) Nutrition Reports International, 1, 131 Meister, A. (1965) Biochemistry of the Amino Acid, Vol. I & II, 2nd ed., Academic Press Meister, A., Bukenberger, M. W. & Strassburger, M. (1963) Biochem. Z., 338, 217 Morselli, P. L. & Garattini, S. (1970) Nature, 227, 611-612 Müller, H. (1970) Z. Ernährungswissensch., 10, 83-88 Murakami, U. & Inouye, M. (1971) Cong. Anom., 11, 171 Mushahwar, I. K. & Koeppe, R. E. (1971) Biochim. Biophys. Acta, 244, 318-321 Neame, K. D. & Wiseman, G. (1957) J. Physiol., 135, 442 Naeme, K. D. & Wiseman, G. (1958) J. Physiol., 140, 148 Ohara, Y., Fujimoto, T., Ichimura, M. & Kirimura, J. (1970a) Unpublished report of Central Research Lab., Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Ohara, Y., Hasegawa, Y., Ichimura, M. & Kirimura, J. (1970b) Unpublished report of Central Research Laboratory, Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Olney, J. W. (1969a) J. Neuropath. Exp. Neurol., 28, 455 Olney, J. W. (1969b) Science, 164, 719 Olney, J. W. (1970b) Unpublished report Olney, J. W. (1971) Science, 172, 294 Olney, J. W., Sharpe, L. G. & Feigin, R. D. (1972) 31, 464 Olney, J. W. & Ho, O. L. (1970) Nature, 227, 609 Olney, J. W., Ho, O. L. & Rhee, V. (1971) Exp. Brain Res., 14, 61 Olney, J. W. & Sharp, L. G. (1969) Science, 166, 386 Oser, B. L., Bailey, D. E., Morgareidge, K., Carson, S. & Vogin, E. E. (1973) Unpublished report submitted to International Glutamate Technical Committee Oser, B. L., Carson, S., Vogin, E. E. & Cox, G. E. (1971) Nature, 229, 411-413 Pagliari, A. S. & Goodman, A. D. (1969) New Eng. J. Med., 281, 767 Peraino, C. & Harper, A. E. (1962) Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 97, 442 Perez, V. J. & Olney, J. W. (1972) J. Neurochem., 19, 1777 Perrault, M. & Dry, J. (1961) Sem. Therapeutique, 37, 597 Peters, J. H., Lin, S. C., Berridge, B. J. jr, Cummings, J. G. & Chao, W. R. (1969) Proc. Soc. expt. Biol. Med., 131, 281 Pinto-Scognamiglio, W., Amorico, L. & Gatti, G. L. (1972) Il Farmaco (Prat), 27, 19-27 Prabhu, V. G. & Oester, Y. T. (1971) Arch. Int. Pharmacodyn. Ther., 189, 59-71 Pradhan, S. N. & Lynch, J. F. jr (1972) Arch. Int. Pharmacodyn., 197, 301 Price, J. C., Waelsch, H. & Putmann, F. (1943) J. Amer. Med. Ass., 122, 1153 Porter, D. B. & Griffin, A. C. (1950) J. Comp. Phys. Psych., 43, i Potts, A. M., Modrell, R. W. & Kingsbury, C. (1960) Amer. J. Ophthal., 50, 900 Reynolds, W. A., Lemkey-Johnston, N., Filer, L. J. jr & Pitkin, R. M. (1971) Science, 172, 1342 Reynolds, W. A. & Lemkey-Johnston, N. (1973) Unpublished data submitted to International Glutamate Technical Committee Roberts, E. & Frankel, S. (1951) J. Biol. Chem., 188, 189 Rosenblum, I., Bradley, J. D. & Coulston, F. (1971) Toxicol. & Appl. Pharmacol., 18, 367-373 Sauberlich, H. E. (1961) J. Nutr., 75, 61 Schaumburg, H. H., Byck, R., Gerstl, R. & Mashman, J. H. (1969) Science, 163, 826 Semprini, M. E., Frasca, M. A. & Mariani, A. (1971) Quaderni della Nutrizione, 31, 85 Shimizu, T. & Aibara, K. (1970) Unpublished report Smyth, C. J., Levey, S. & Lasichak, A. G. (1947) Amer. J. Med. Sci., 214, 281 Stegink, L. D. Filer, L. J. jr & Baker, G. L. (1972) Proc. Soc. Expt. Biol. & Med., 140, 836 Stegink, L. G., Pitkin, R. M., Reynolds, W. A. & Filer, L. J. jr (1973) Federation Proceedings, 32, Abstract 3797 Stellar, E. & McElroy, W. D. (1948) Science, 108, 281 Sugahara, M. & Ariyoshi, S. (1967) Agr. Biol. Chem., 31, 1270-1275 Suzuki, Y. & Takahashi, M. (1970) Unpublished report to Ajinomoto Co. Inc. Tugrul, S. (1965) Arch. int. Pharmacodyn., 153, 323 U.K. Dept. of Health & Soc. Security (1970) Unpublished food survey tables U.S. Food & Drug Administration, Bureau of Science-Bureau of Medicine (1969) Report on monosodium glutamate for review by Food Protection Committee, NAS/NRC, Washington, D.C. Waelsch, H. (1949) Lancet, i, 257 Walshe, J. M. (1953) Lancet. i, 1075 Weil-Malherbe, H. (1936) Biochem. J., 30, 665 Wen, C. P., Hayes, K. C. & Gershoff, S. N. (1973) J. Nutr. (In press) Wheeler, P. & Morgan, A. F. (1958) J. Nutr., 64, 137 Wilson, W. E. & Koeppe, R. E. (1959) J. Biol. Chem., 234, 1186 Wilson, W. E. & Koeppe, R. E. (1961) J. Biol. Chem., 236, 365 Wiseman, G. (1970) Unpublished report Yamamura, Y. (1960) Med. J. Shinshu Univ., 5, 1 Yanagisawa, K., Nakamura, T., Miyata, K., Kameda, T., Kitamura, S. & Ito, K. (1961) Nohon Seirigaku Zasshi (J. Physiol. Soc. Japan, 23, 383) Yonetani, S. (1967) Unpublished report of Central Research Laboratories, Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Yonetani, S., Ishii, H. & Kirimura, J. (1970) Unpublished report of Central Research Laboratories, Ajinomoto Co., Inc. Zimmerman, F. T. & Burgemeister, B. B. (1959) A.M.A. Arch. Neut. Psych., 81, 639
See Also: Toxicological Abbreviations